Introduction

Within language exists a conglomerate of hypothesized ‘dead ends’ or ‘language myths’ (Hall, Smith, and Wicaksono, 2011) as they are also known. A series of shared, widespread language assumptions that hold little merit under linguistic scrutiny. Yet, how does something become a ‘dead end’ and how is one defined? To define a dead end, one needs to analyse how they impede progression in the face of understanding language use and base language ideology. They are called ‘Dead Ends’ for a reason, after all. This portfolio will aim to take a sample of those myths, picking them apart for the end goal of better understanding the aforementioned questions and better understanding of universal language use in general. 

Myth 2: 'Written language is superior to spoken language'

Written language is a phenomenon that has only recently become a widely available thing. Humans have always had ways of recording their thoughts, experiences and stories, dating all the way back to when ancient humans painted on cave walls to the modern day where pen and paper and computers are widely used. Hall, Smith and Wicaksono (2011) go so far as to state that written language is most likely one of the most important human developments in last few millennia’s. While written language is not a recent development by a more short-term measure, it is only within the last couple of centuries that public education has allowed it to be available to the masses. Prior to this, written language has always been a possession of the ‘elite’. This could be why the more ‘standard’ form of the language, the kind that we find in written language, is heralded as ‘superior’ in a spoken context. Linell (2005, p.10) also suggests that the links of written language to the Church during its height of power is a contributing factor. However, to make this assumption of language would be wrong. Written language is merely a method of communication and a form of documenting language. Both spoken language and written language bear their own merits and advantages, both worthy of their own regard and study. Linell (2005), throughout his work, speaks of the language bias to written language by both the common person and linguists, too. This shows how deeply rooted this myth is in our society and why it is worthy of intense scrutiny and mention.

 

 

 

 

Myth 1: Usage of language a reflection of intelligence?

The first myth is the belief that the way in which someone uses their language reflects upon their intelligence. Now, as a myth, one might assume that this one bears merit after observing modern society and social connotations. In some ways, it does. However, one must have a clear definition of intelligence. Is intelligence the ability to recall clear-cut rules established throughout education? Or, is it more mental ability: the scope and potential of an individual's thoughts and perspective? Shakespeare was famous for his liberal take on the English language, his notoriety for coining new terms unparalleled throughout the language, yet he is also heralded as a master of the quill - one would not assume him to be lacking in intelligence simply because he did not stick to the dictation of language at the time. Another, more contemporary, example to be held in argument for this point is that which Hall, Smith and Wicaksono highlight: the modern day IQ tests. IQ is commonly held as an indicator of one’s intelligence, the measuring unit. Then why is it that these tests are often comprised of mathematical, orthographical or grammar-related questions?  Hall, Smith, and Wicaksono (2011, p.10) state “None of these attributes are present in the test-taker because of their intelligence.” The work of Bernstein (1960) also supports this view as he denotes that the role of intelligence in the use of language is the ability to exploit more successfully the wider forms of language use, not in that language use in itself is a bold indicator of one’s intelligence. Bernstein makes this analysis as part of a broader series of research around the subject of language and social class – a possible source of the myth. As was previously mentioned, observing social connotations and modern society, it is easy to append a certain type of speech to a ‘social class’, in turn attaching the stereotypes of that class to that manner of diction.

Myth 3: 'Some groups of people don't use their language properly'

The final myth this portfolio will cover is that, ‘some groups of people don’t use their language properly’. Hall, Smith and Wicaksono outline this with the theory that a ‘standard language’ has to be present for the perceived improper use to be pertinent. How do we then define this standard language? One might argue that ‘standard language’ is as we have discussed in the myth regarding written language: that it is the tongue of the elite and almost like a centre of power. In English, we can observe this that the common colloquialism “the Queen’s English” is in reference to the Received Pronunciation and standard form. However, if one is to assume that this is the ‘correct’ form, then what is it that makes any deviating form incorrect? Certainly, a standard form persists as this is what we use as a baseline. It is what children are marked on in school, what legal documents are penned in, what even most novels are constructed in. This becomes a ‘dead end’, however, when one starts to perceive any deviating form as problematic and wrong. Take, for example, the presence of regional accents and the assumptions people make on those who speak with them; or, more broadly, those from outside the UK who speak with altered forms of English. At one point, it would be unheard of for the BBC to broadcast a news report with the presenter bearing even a slightly regional accent. If one form of English is perceived to be superior, to be ‘correct’, then this distinctly hinders the progress of the study of language, the teaching of language and even the evolution of the language itself. The attached video, while being a comedy skit, is an example of this myth in popular culture.

Conclusion

In reflection of the myths posed by Hall, Smith and Wicaksono (2011), with the three covered, we can see that while many of them bear contemporary, social merit in that they exist, all of them are highly disadvantageous in regards to the progression of linguistics and our understanding of language. It would be highly prudent that all linguists be aware of these ‘dead ends’, lest they fall into these traps leading their work to be held with a similar regard. In relation to the department of teaching, mostly so. Should someone in the education profession take to thinking that a student was less intelligent for the slang terms they used, or that they were somehow lesser of a student because they could not write well or that they did not have a good grasp on ‘standard language’, then they would be doing that student a huge disservice. It is upon reflection of these stated ‘dead ends’ that we can really analyse our own beliefs on language and our interactions with people from all walks of life from here on in. To acknowledge and understand language and to be mindful of these pitfalls would make everyone a better linguist. 

References

  • Bernstein, B. (1960) Language and Social Class, The British Journal of Sociology, 11(3), pp. 271-176
  • Hall, C.J., Smith, P.H. and Wicaksono, R. (2011) Mapping applied linguistics: A guide for students and practitioners. Abingdon: Taylor & Francis.
  • Linell, Per. (2005) The Written Language Bias In Linguistics. 1st ed. London: Routledge,